>Users:   login   |  register       > email     > people    


Mouthy Offenders And Violence
By Leonard A. Sipes, Jr.
Published: 04/02/2018

Inmate leaning on cell I grew up in Baltimore where you clearly understood that street behaviors had consequences.

As a child, I rode my bike fifteen blocks to play baseball which meant that I traveled through neighborhoods that weren’t mine. That meant that I would encounter kids who didn’t know me. That meant that territory matters.

If I kept to the main streets, as advised by my father, I would be OK. But if I strayed, it meant that I was entering unknown territory where I would be challenged by other kids. I did, they did, and I got mildly beat for being mouthy in the wrong place.

My father’s response? “I told you to stick to the main street.” You got beat because you were stupid.”

Needless to say, the police were not called. The fault was mine. I didn’t adhere to street realities. My verbal responses were too aggressive. It was a lesson learned.

Why Verbal Encounters Turn Violent

Some in the justice system view criminology as a restatement of the obvious using terms we don’t understand; the research below from the National Institute of Justice fits this notion.

It examines why encounters turn violent, and it has implications for cops, parole and probation agents, correctional officers and anyone who deals with people caught up in the justice system.

Any cop or resident of a high crime city can tell you how encounters turn violent and offer accurate observations about the offenders involved.

When I frequented the bars of Baltimore, I always apologized when I bumped into someone (or he bumped into me) knowing he could be carrying a weapon and was probably drunk or high or may have a history of violence.

“Sorry man,” came instinctively and his “no problem,” response was automatic. It was just better to defuse the encounter immediately.

But I saw fights where no one backed down, the well-understood niceties were not observed, his friends challenged him to stand up for himself, and someone got beat.

Research Findings (my interpretation of data)

Based on the five research papers below examining the dynamics of street criminal violence:

Verbal disputes involving offenders and non-offenders are likely to be different. Offenders are more likely to be verbally aggressive and less likely to engage in remedial actions.

Offenders are also more likely to be intoxicated and their aggression is more likely to have the support of third parties. These tendencies may explain why disputes involving offenders are more likely to escalate to violence, and why offenders are more likely to become victims.

The results suggest that: (1) not being able to control your mouth (disputatiousness) accounts for a substantial portion of the relationship between offending and victimization; (2) offenders tend to be disputatious because of their low self-control, heavy alcohol use, and honor-related attitudes; and (3) the primary reason men with low self-control and alcohol problems are more likely to become victims is their disputatiousness.

The evidence suggested that verbal disputes with antagonists who are violent offenders are more likely to become violent because offenders are: (1) more likely to make threats; (2) less likely to engage in remedial actions; (3) more likely to be intoxicated; and (4) more likely to receive encouragement to be aggressive by third parties.

When present, third parties were most likely to encourage aggression and least likely to mediate disputes between relationally distant men.

In sum, offenders are more likely to experience a verbal attack, interpret comments as attacks, and respond to verbal attacks with aggression.

Note that in my hundreds of conversations with offenders, few would dispute what’s offered above. They often see violence as necessary, good and something that protects themselves, their property and their loved ones.

We see violence as a statistic. They see a reality where very aggressive and chaotic behaviors are often in their best interest.

Report (edited for readability)

One of the strongest empirical associations in the criminological literature is between offending and victimization. Yet not much is known about the mechanisms that produce the victim-offender overlap. Largely absent from this literature is research on the situational context of disputes.

Much can be learned about the etiology of the overlap by examining why offenders are more likely to be victimized in disputes. The purpose of our project was to examine explanations of the victim-offender overlap using data obtained from male prison inmates and a community sample.

No other data exists, to our knowledge, that allows for the analyses of 1) disputatiousness and its relationship to the overlap, 2) and the situational characteristics of interpersonal disputes that contribute to the overlap.

The data gathered for this project allow for both analyses. Project Design and Methods Our sample includes male inmates from the Pennsylvania state prison system and men they knew from the community who had never been arrested.

Respondents were asked about the frequency of their verbal conflicts and their violent victimizations. They were also asked questions measuring their self-control, alcohol use, and honor-based attitudes. These measures were used for our individual-level analyses.

Our incident analyses were based on questions in which respondents were asked about two types of disputes that occurred before they were incarcerated. One was a dispute involving verbal aggression and the other a dispute involving violence. Respondents were asked whether, during these disputes, they or their antagonists engaged in insults, threats, remedial actions (e.g., apologies) and whether they were intoxicated.

We also asked about the content of the dispute including whether it was related to their involvement in illegal business activity.

We also asked whether there were third parties were present and whether they engaged in mediation and instigation during the incident.

Finally, we asked whether the antagonist had a criminal record and whether the antagonist had a reputation for violence.

Our project has resulted in five papers so far, two of them published. We describe them below.

“An Appraisal of Research on the Victim-Offender Overlap” Our theoretical approach is described in our first published paper from the project (Berg and Felson 2016). We offered a situational explanation of the overlap based on a social interactionist perspective on aggression (Tedeschi and Felson 1994). A social interactionist approach suggests that, since most homicides and assaults stem from verbal disputes, it is important to examine the social interaction between adversaries and third parties during those disputes that lead to escalation.

We argued that the characteristics of the verbal disputes involving offenders and non-offenders are likely to be different. Specifically, we hypothesized that offenders are more likely to be verbally aggressive and less likely to engage in remedial actions.

Offenders are also more likely to be intoxicated and their aggression is more likely to have the support of third parties, given the social contexts they tend to inhabit. These tendencies may explain why disputes involving offenders are more likely to escalate to violence, and why offenders are more likely to become victims.

“Disputatiousness and the Offender-Victim Overlap” This paper was published in the Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency. Additionally, we presented results from this paper at the 2016 Annual Conference for the American Society of Criminology.

The paper focuses on an individual difference analysis, not an incident analysis. We examine whether offenders are at greater risk of violent victimization because of their disputatiousness, i.e., their tendency to become involved in verbal conflicts.

We also examine whether offenders are more disputatious because of their low self-control, alcohol use, and honor-based attitudes and whether disputatiousness can explain the effect of these individual differences on violent victimization.

The results suggest that: (1) disputatiousness accounts for a substantial portion of the relationship between offending and victimization; (2) offenders tend to be disputatious because of their low self-control, heavy alcohol use, and honor-related attitudes; and (3) the primary reason men with low self-control and alcohol problems are more likely to become victims is their disputatiousness.

“A Social Interactionist Approach to the Victim-Offender Overlap” We plan to submit this paper to the Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency in the next few weeks. In the paper, we used incident data to test hypotheses about the victim-offender overlap suggested by the social interactionist perspective.

Specifically, we examined whether the tendency for offenders to become involved in violent disputes and to be victims of violence reflected their provocative behavior, their alcohol use, and their failure to engage in remedial actions during the disputes they reported.

We also examined whether offenders were more likely to be victimized because third parties who were present during their disputes were more likely to support escalation and less likely to act as mediators.

The design involved an examination of whether the characteristics of disputes mediated the relationship between whether the antagonist was an offender and violent outcomes. The violent outcomes (our dependent variables) were whether the respondent physically attacked the antagonist and whether the dispute involved violence or not. Here is a depiction of the design:

Antagonist has a criminal record.

Antagonist’s behavior during the dispute.

Antagonist is assaulted by respondent (or the dispute included violence).

The evidence suggested that verbal disputes with antagonists who are violent offenders are more likely to become violent because offenders are: (1) more likely to make threats; (2) less likely to engage in remedial actions; (3) more likely to be intoxicated; and (4) more likely to receive encouragement to be aggressive by third parties.

The results suggest that approximately half of the victim-offender overlap is due to the way offenders and third parties behave during verbal disputes.

“Effects of Gender and the Relationship between Adversaries on the Involvement of Third Parties in Disputes” In this unpublished paper, we use the incident data to examine when third parties are present during the disputes and how they behave if they are present.

We argue that third parties can play an important role in determining whether disputes escalate to violence.

Third parties may mediate the dispute or they may act as instigator or allies. Even if they do nothing, they may serve as an audience and increase the adversaries’ concerns for self-image. This paper focuses on the effects of gender and the relationship between adversaries (relational distance) on the presence and behavior of third parties.

We hypothesize that a privacy norm inhibits intervention in disputes involving couples or families or perhaps any close relationship.

We compare these relationships to relationships between acquaintances and strangers. Third parties may be less likely to be present when relational distance is close and, when they are present, they may not become involved.

The privacy norm may be particularly important in disputes involving intimate partners. On the other hand, the effects of privacy may be offset by a norm inhibiting aggression against women. “Chivalry” may produce the “behind closed door effect” where violence against women is hidden. When third parties are present, they may attempt to protect women. They may engage in more mediation, less instigation, and more partisanship on behalf of women.

In general, our evidence suggests that third parties are most likely to be present during men’s disputes with relationally distant men (strangers and acquaintances) and least likely to be present during their disputes with women.

When present, third parties were most likely to encourage aggression and least likely to mediate disputes between relationally distant men.

Third parties were more likely to take the women’s side in inter-gender disputes. Disputes involving distant males were more likely include partisans on both sides.

“Bones of Contention: Precipitants of Disputes among Offenders and Non-offenders” We examined differences in the precipitants of disputes involving violent and non-violent offenders and non-offenders. This involves a comparison of disputes involving inmates with self-reported violence, other inmates, men from the community sample with self-reported violence, and other men from the community.

We analyzed both verbal disputes and violent disputes.

We presented this paper at the 2017 Annual Conference for the American Society of Criminology and are currently revising the paper for publication.

We made four hypotheses about the precipitants of these disputes. First, we hypothesized that the disputes of offenders are more likely than the disputes of non-offenders to stem from their tendency to provoke grievances. Offenders are more likely to get into disputes because they violate norms and provoke social control responses from others.

For example, offenders tend to have unstable work histories, financial problems, conflicts with their families, and substance abuse issues. Their poor role performance and misbehavior can lead others to attempt to control them. These attempts at informal social control can lead to disputes involving verbal aggression or violence.

Thus we expected their disputes to be more likely to begin when offenders violate norms, when someone criticizes them or gives them a command, or when they do not comply with requests. An alternative perspective is that offenders get into disputes when they try to control others.

Their violence reflects an attempt to dominate an adversary. In that case, one would expect their disputes to be precipitated by their control behavior and their adversaries’ violations.

Our second hypothesis was based on the literature on the role of identity conflicts and honor in violent encounters. We hypothesize that the disputes of offenders are more likely than the disputes of non-offenders to stem from verbal attacks from adversaries.

Evidence suggests that offenders are more likely to have a hostility bias: they interpret the behavior of others as having aggressive intent when it does not (Dodge 1980).

In addition, offenders are more likely to retaliate when they are verbally attacked. They are more concerned with honor and with showing strength to prevent future attack (e.g., Anderson 1999).

Finally, offenders are more likely to get into disputes because they actually experience more frequent verbal attacks. The opportunity for verbal attack may be greater because they are more likely to associate with people who are aggressive or because their routine activities expose them to aggressive people.

In sum, offenders are more likely to experience verbal attack, interpret comments as attacks, and respond to verbal attacks with aggression.

Third, we hypothesized that the disputes of offenders are more likely to stem from their participation in illegal business activity. Illegal business activity is conducive to violence because participants lack legal recourse when they have grievances (Black 1976).

As a result, offenders turn to self-help strategies, which often involve violence. In addition, it is reasonable to assume that the type of people involved in illegal businesses are less likely to be honest in their dealings and more likely to use aggression.

Finally, we examined whether offenders are more or less likely to intervene on behalf of third parties. One image of offenders is that they are particularly selfish and narcissistic. They cheat, lie, and steal when it is in their interest. Thus psychopaths are described as callous and lacking of empathy.

This characterization implies that their disputes are less likely to involve their intervention on behalf of others. When they get involved in disputes they are acting on their own behalf.

Paul Bloom’s recent discussion of empathy and violence implies the opposite. He argues that the tendency to feel someone else’s pain can lead to violence against people who produce that suffering.

People are particularly likely to empathize with members of their group and this emotion leads them to aggressive behavior against an out group.

Incident level analyses of precipitants of verbal and violent disputes generally support these hypotheses.

We also find that offenders’ participation in disputes is more likely to involve their intervention on behalf of third parties. This evidence is consistent with recent claims that empathy sometimes leads to violence on behalf of an in-group.

Source: National Institute of Justice

Reprinted with permission from http://www.crimeinamerica.net.

Contact us at crimeinamerica@gmail.com or for media on deadline, use leonardsipes@gmail.com.

Leonard A. Sipes, Jr has thirty-five years of experience supervising public affairs for national and state criminal justice agencies. He is the Former Senior Specialist for Crime Prevention for the Department of Justice’s clearinghouse and the Former Director of Information Management for the National Crime Prevention Council. He has a Post Master’s degree from Johns Hopkins University and is the author of the book "Success With the Media". He can be reached via email at leonardsipes@gmail.com.


Comments:

  1. Aourt on 05/31/2019:

    I am glad you were able to share this experience. I always say there is more to a picture. We all have done things that were are not proud for big nose. I have made some mistakes. The goal is to learn from them and move on. The mistakes and experinces made me what I am today. I do not regret anything.


Login to let us know what you think

User Name:   

Password:       


Forgot password?





correctsource logo




Use of this web site constitutes acceptance of The Corrections Connection User Agreement
The Corrections Connection ©. Copyright 1996 - 2024 © . All Rights Reserved | 15 Mill Wharf Plaza Scituate Mass. 02066 (617) 471 4445 Fax: (617) 608 9015